Framing bias in decision-making entails how the way information is presented can be crucial in order to influence people to make a different decision or change a perspective, rendering wrongly framed information ineffective. The psychological experiments mentioned in this article explore the framing bias further.

One of the first psychologists to explore the framing bias in decision-making were Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman. Their experiments were often quite simple yet fascinating. During them, Kahneman and Tversky asked people to imagine themselves in various hypothetical scenarios and make decisions. Below you will read about a few of them that are the most relevant in regards to the framing bias.
1st experiment: Choices that highlight gains make people risk averse while choices about losses make people risk seeking
Over 300 participants were introduced to a hypothetical scenario where scientists had determined that a dangerous disease was expected to affect 600 people. Scientists proposed two plans to prevent the disease. Participants were split into two groups. In the first group, the plans focused on gains:
- If plan A is adopted, 200 people will be saved.
- If plan B is adopted, there is 1/3 probability that 600 people will be saved, and 2/3 probability that no people will be saved.
During this scenario, 72 percent of the participants choose plan A, which was the option with clear gains. Now let's look at the second group. Now the plans were focused on losses:
- If plan C is adopted 400 people will die.
- If plan D is adopted there is 1/3 probability that nobody will die, and 2/3 probability that 600 people will die.
The second group acted in a completely different way. Merely changing the wording to focus on losses made the participants choose the riskier second option 78 percent of the time (compared to 28 percent in the first group that choose this option).
The results show how by framing the same problem differently while providing the same information, the outcomes can be changed dramatically.
2nd experiment: Investigating gains VS. losses in relation to monetary bets
The second experiment was a bit different. This time a group of 150 participants was asked to make 2 decisions instead of only showing one of the decisions to two groups. The first decision focused on gains and asked participants to choose between:
- A sure gain of $240
- 25% chance to gain $1000, and 75% chance to gain nothing
84 percent of the participants chose the first option, repeating the same behavior as in the previous experiment when evaluating gains. Now let's look at the second choice that the participants had to make:
- A sure loss of $750
- 75% chance to lose $1000, and 25% chance to lose nothing
This time 87 percent of the participants chose the riskier, second option which shows how we evaluate risk-taking based on if we are thinking about the decision as a potential gain or a potential loss.
3rd experiment: The use of certainty effect while framing choices
Certainty effect states that people value 100% certain outcomes significantly more than outcomes with a probability or range of options. That's what the third experiment is about. Over 150 participants had to make a choice:
- A sure win of $30
- 80%o chance to win $45
Even though value-wise, the second option is way more profitable, the majority (78 percent) of the participants chose the first option. Let's not just write it off as the risk aversion due to possible gains. The second part of the experiment had people make a similar choice but this time, with no certain gains choice:
- 25% chance to win $30
- 20%o chance to win $45
Even though the second option has 20 percent lower odds of winning, just like in the first decision (from 100 to 80 percent), this time 58 percent chose it (compared to only 22 percent in the first decision).
The results of the experiment show that we value certainty but when the odds are low, we tend to go for a higher risk and higher reward option which can be used while framing choices to influence people to choose one over the other.
Conclusion
Framing bias depends on various other psychological effects for its work. In this article, we explored how people choose a risk-free option when thinking about gains but when judging losses or low odds, the risk-taking approach becomes the most popular one. This indicates that framing the problem as gains or losses can influence the way people will react to it.
Subscribe to our monthly newsletter!
More articles
- Default effect (behavioral economics): Default options influence your decisions
- Wisdom of crowds theory: Do crowds make better decisions?
- Anchoring in behavioral economics: How unrelated information influences your choices
- How the decoy effect in marketing makes you spend more
- Familiarity effect: How people prefer familiarity over novelty